A Response to Cultural Materialism: The
Struggle for a Science of Culture by Marvin Harris
Marvin Harris's argument against what he terms "phenomenological
obscurantism" is that science can do much more than the Boasian
anthropologists give it credit for in the delineation of cultures. In
fact, Harris contends, "phenomenological obscurantism" leads to
"intellectual suicide." He cites the multiple takes on the story in the
film Roshomon. He contends that, though each story is viewed
through the subjective impressions of its storyteller, the filmmaker could
just as well have taken the common elements of each story and created a
version that everyone would have to admit was the truth. His point is that
the methods of science are applicable trans-culturally, and that we do not
have to accept the vagaries of subjective interpretation alone in the
establishment of truth. There are several major flaws in his argument. I'm
going to look at three of them.
- The methods of science are applicable trans-culturally
. Of
course, this is true when dealing with physical properties, but it isn't
so clear-cut when examining culture itself. At about the same time that
Harris was writing his study, a survey was being conducted in Southern
California of Japanese women who had married American soldiers stationed
in Japan. Bilingual interviewers met with the same women on two
occasions. The first interviews were conducted in Japanese. The second,
taken several months later, were conducted in English. Though the
questions asked in both interviews were identical, the answers differed
radically depending upon the language in which they were asked. For
example, to the question in Japanese, "What happens when you and your
husband disagree about something?" one woman responded "It is a time of
great difficulty for us." When the same woman was later asked the
question in English, her answer was "I do whatever I want." Similar
contrasting responses characterized the survey. What accounts for this?
The study concluded that beyond subjective or objective reactions to it,
culture itself, through the vehicle of language, determines a great
degree of practical reality.
- The methods of science and of "Boasian" anthropologists are
opposed.
Franz Boas, as I've written on a couple of occasions, was a
physicist who became an anthropologist because he believed that the
science he knew could not answer the questions that his experiences in
the field demanded. If we believe (as Harris seems to take as a given)
that science is monolithic and purely objective, then we have to
conclude that Boas was a poor scientist. However, the science that Boas
knew was limited by two factors. First, Boas was a student of Wundt and
Fechner, who were the founders of psychophysics, which posited the
physical measurement of human perception--a formal methodology that
failed Boas in his experiences with the Inuit on Baffin Island. Second,
the culture of science in 19th century Germany took for granted that
European culture was the highest and best on earth and that Germany was
at the pinnacle of European culture. Boas' field experiences shattered
the notion of cultural superiority for him utterly. Boas' anthropology
was a response to the limited science available to him. Like science,
anthropology is not static--or shouldn't be. Both attempt to answer
questions, but neither alone has yet proved adequate in answering all
the questions.
- "Phenomenological obscurantism" leads to "intellectual suicide."
In theory, Harris makes a good argument against the futility and
self-referential spinning of the phenomenologists. But only in a
theoretical sense. In practice, much good method has developed since
Harris wrote his book--method that Harris' argument denies. Here are two
examples: First, the work of Richard Bandler and John Grinder, authors
of The Structure of Magic books. In these studies the authors
observed some of the world's most talented psychotherapists in action
and distilled the similarities of their techniques--despite their
divergent intuitive approaches--into usable knowledge. These studies
have been instrumental in the techniques of NLP. Second, the
"utilization-focused" research methodology pioneered by Michael Quinn
Patton and others. This is a collection of techniques researchers can
bring to the understanding of the needs of institutions and their
administrators in order to effect practical change. The methods employ
both "scientific" modeling and "phenomenological" observation in order
to arrive at a concrete set of procedures that will bring about the
desired ends. I emphasize that the works of Bandler and Grinder, and of
Michael Quinn Patton are reality-based. That is, the proof of their
validity is in their continued effectiveness in the practical world.
Harris may have proved them invalid in theory, but in reality they've
left him far behind.
--Sandy McIntosh
|
|